Mackenzie Bergstrom

For her PhD dissertation, Mackenzie Bergstrom of the University of Calgary studied 25 adult female capuchins living in three habituated social groups in a tropical dry forest in Sector Santa Rosa (SSR) of the Área de Conservaciόn Guanacaste (ACG) in northwest Costa Rica. To better understand how ecological and social variables affect the physical condition of these New World monkeys, the goals of this project were to document the dietary profile of these females, measure the extent of the seasonal variation in diet and nutritional intake, determine if seasonal variation in the availability of foods affects the physical condition of females and determine ecological and social correlates of energy balance and stress using urinary C-peptide and fecal cortisol.

Female white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus

Bergstrom conducted 12 months of behavioral observations, phenological surveys and nutritional analyses of food items consumed, radio-immunoassays of urinary c-peptide of insulin as a measure of energetic condition, and enzyme-immunoassays of fecal cortisol as a measure of stress. In the report below she describes the results of her project, providing insight into behavioral strategies employed by females at varying reproductive states and social ranks in response to proximate ecological and social pressures.  


Posted
AuthorH Gregory

Samantha Porter in the lithics lab at the University of Minnesota

The next grantee from our Fall 2014 granting cycle is Samantha Porter. She is a PhD candidate from the University of Minnesota, Twin Cities, and her project is entitled "Investigating cultural transmission across the Middle to Upper Paleolithic transition in Western Europe."

Around 40,000 years ago, anatomically modern humans began migrating out of Africa into Western Europe.  Europe was not empty prior to this migration, but was already inhabited by our close evolutionary cousins, the Neanderthals.

Samantha Porter demonstrating how to make 3D models using photogrammetry

My research tests for evidence of cultural exchange between Neanderthals and anatomically modern humans during this tumultuous time in prehistory. It is based on the principle that social proximity between groups has an effect on the way groups apply knowledge they've learned from one another. My research hypothesizes that if groups are interacting closely, individuals will be able to learn all stages of a technological production process from start to finish. For stone tools, this encompasses both the creation of flake ‘blanks’ and the reshaping of these blanks to produce formal tools. Alternatively, if two groups are interacting with each other in a more socially distant fashion, perhaps only finding material traces of each other left behind on the pathways of the landscape, a group acquiring a new technology from another is more likely to use their own blank production methods and then attempt to replicate only the form of the final product or tool of the other group.

Samantha Porter measuring lithic artifacts in the Musée national de préhistoire in Les-Eyzies-de-Tayac, France

In short, by looking at similarities and differences between assemblages of artifacts associated with Neanderthal and anatomically modern human groups at different steps of the stone tool making process, I not only seek to find evidence of the presence or absence of cultural exchanges between modern human and Neanderthal groups but also gain a better understanding of how closely these groups may have interacted with one another.

Posted
AuthorH Gregory
2 CommentsPost a comment

Our understanding of human evolution has grown exponentially since Darwin's time. This week marks the 206th anniversary of the birth of Charles Darwin, so we're sharing a Darwin-related Leakey Foundation lecture from our archives. In this lecture, recorded in 2009 at the Field Museum in Chicago, Daniel Lieberman of Harvard University discusses the evolution and dysevolution of humans 150 years after On the Origin of Species.

The Leakey Foundation and the Field Museum present Dr. Dan Lieberman at The Field Museum. April 4, 2009. Full Title: Survival of the Swiftest, Smartest or Fastest: Human Evolution 150 Years After Darwin


Posted
AuthorMeredith Johnson

The next fall 2014 grantee we would like to introduce to you is Maura Tyrrell. She is a PhD candidate from the University at Buffalo, State University of New York, and her dissertation project is entitled "Effect of competition on male coalition patterns in crested macaques."

Maura Tyrrell and a crested macaque

My dissertation focuses on the social relationships between wild adult male crested macaques (Macaca nigra) at Tangkoko Nature Reserve, Indonesia. I am specifically examining coalition behavior between males in different competitive contexts.

Competition between groups of related and unrelated males plays a large role in the political structure of early humans. However, current theoretical explanations of male coalitions in nonhuman primates focus primarily on mate competition within the group and seldom consider the influence of competition between groups. With my study I hope to create a clearer picture of social relationships in crested macaques by incorporating between-group and within-group competitive contexts into my examination of coalition and relationship qualities. I want to know if competitive context of coalitions (i.e. whether coalitions are directed towards a resident or male or an immigrating male/outside male in a neighboring group) varies with behaviors proposed to manage conflicts between individuals (i.e. "reconciling" after a fight, signaling friendly intentions, ritualized "greetings"). Additionally, does coalition partner choice in each context reflect differentiated patterns of affiliation, or is it primarily opportunistic?

The answers to these questions should help broaden our perspective of the evolution of human male coalitions by identifying components of early human alliances that may have evolved before the common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees. Findings of parallels between coalition patterns in crested macaques, chimpanzees, and humans will suggest that these parallels not only pre-date the common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees, but that they do not require male philopatry and clusters of related males. 

Posted
AuthorH Gregory

Leakey Foundation grantees Israel Hershkovitz and Ofer Marder led an international team of archaeologists who discovered a 55,000 year old cranium in Manot Cave in Israel. Their discovery was described last week in the journal Nature

Photo courtesy of : Clara Amit, Israel Antiquities Authority

Photo courtesy of : Clara Amit, Israel Antiquities Authority

A key event in human evolution was the expansion of modern humans of African origin across Eurasia, replacing all other forms of hominin (humans and their predecessors) around 40,000-60,000 years ago. However, due to the scarcity of human fossils from this period, the path these ancestors took as they expanded out of Africa has largely remained a mystery.

Interior of Manot Cave. Photo courtesy of Amos Frumkin / Hebrew University Cave Research Center

Interior of Manot Cave. Photo courtesy of Amos Frumkin / Hebrew University Cave Research Center

The Manot Cave finding provides the first fossil evidence from the critical period when genetic and archaeological models predict that African modern humans successfully migrated out of Africa and populated Eurasia. It is also the first fossil evidence that there were populations of modern humans living near populations of Neanderthals in the Levant during the late Middle Paleolithic. This leads some to speculate that the Manot Cave people could be the population that initially bred with Neanderthals, giving all modern non-African people a little bit of Neanderthal DNA.

The Leakey Foundation recently awarded Ofer Marder of Ben-Gurion University a research grant to fund further excavation of Manot Cave. We are thrilled to be funding the exploration of this important site. We look forward to sharing news of more discoveries. 

The research appears in the journal Nature under the title "Levantine cranium from Manot Cave (Israel) foreshadows the first European modern humans" (DOI 10.1038/nature14134).

Inside the Manot Cave in Israel's Galilee, where a 55,000-year-old skull sheds new light on human migration patterns. Photo courtesy of: Amos Frumkin / Hebrew University Cave Research Center

Inside the Manot Cave in Israel's Galilee, where a 55,000-year-old skull sheds new light on human migration patterns. Photo courtesy of: Amos Frumkin / Hebrew University Cave Research Center

You can learn more about the discoveries at Manot Cave in the following articles:

"Skull Fossil Offers New Clues on Human Journey From Africa", New York Times.

"Neanderthals gain human neighbor", Nature.







Posted
AuthorMeredith Johnson
CategoriesIn the News

Naomi Cleghorn, University of Texas at Arlington, was awarded a Leakey Foundation research grant in the fall of 2014 for her project entitled "Investigating a rare Early Later Stone Age site at Knysna, South Africa."

Naomi Cleghorn at Pinnacle Point site 5/6, Mossel Bay, South Africa

Despite widespread interest in the potential origins of modern human cognitive, social, and technological innovations in the Middle Stone Age (MSA) of Southern Africa, very little is known about how these developed into the suite of strategies exhibited by Holocene and recent foragers of southern Africa. Technologically, much changed between the earlier MSA and the Later Stone Age (LSA) of the Last Glacial Maximum, but the intervening industries are enigmatic and poorly represented. This is in part the result of a substantial drop in the number of documented archaeological sequences dating to the period between 45 kya and 25 kya. In contrast, the archaeological records of Europe and Western Asia document a proliferation of sites after 40 kya. The dearth of sites in South Africa is particularly difficult to reconcile with paleoclimate reconstructions indicating this region enjoyed a relatively mild, wet stadial, and with recent genetic research (Kim et al., 2014) suggesting that the southern African population was the largest on the planet during the Late Pleistocene. Where did these populations go for 20 k years and why did their technology change?

Graduate student Christopher Shelton (L) and Naomi Cleghorn working at Pinnacle Point site 5/6. They found KEH-1 in 2012 during a survey. Shelton is a research assistant on the current Leakey-funded project.

My team and I have recently discovered a site on the southern coast that will help answer these questions. Knysna Eastern Heads Cave 1 (KEH-1) is currently a coastal site, but in the late Pleistocene, it would have had looked south out over a coastal plain up to 75 km wide. KEH-1 is unique in preserving the only archaeological sequence dating between 44 and 18 kya on the southern coast. My international team of scientists and student researchers will expand on the previous test excavation in July and August of 2015 and then use the analyses of stone tools, fauna, plant remains, stable isotopes, and the micromorphology of site stratigraphy to investigate human responses to the changing landscape.

Naomi Cleghorn and Leesha Richardson during the test excavation of Knysa Eastern Heads Cave 1 (KEH-1)

600ft trail, Harkerville Forest, taken during 2012 survey in the Knysna area

Naomi Cleghorn (upper right) and excavation crew at Pinnacle Point

Naomi Cleghorn (upper right) and excavation crew at Pinnacle Point

Posted
AuthorH Gregory

We are happy to introduce another one of our fall 2014 grantees, Shelby S. Putt, PhD candidate from the University of Iowa. Her dissertation project is entitled "Investigating the co-evolution of language and toolmaking:  An fNIRS study."

Shelby S. Putt

Our language and cognition are arguably the features that most distinguish us from other species, and yet, we still know so little about the evolution of these features. As products of evolution, our brains may hold clues to the ancient cognitive and linguistic capabilities of our distant human ancestors. For example, how our brains process the same stone toolmaking behaviors that early hominins practiced may be the closest we ever come to observing extinct hominin cognition in action.

There is evidence to suggest that language instruction reorganizes the cognitive networks involved in learning to make stone tools (Putt et al. 2014). For this reason my team and I are exploring how the act of making stone tools (i.e., flintknapping) is processed differently in the brain depending on whether it was learned verbally or nonverbally. This is an important question, as early Homo probably did not possess fully modern language and also may not have actively taught their young how to flintknap.

Research assistants Chloe Daniel and Danielle Jones demonstrate the setup for collecting data with fNIRS.

My research employs a cutting-edge brain imaging technique known as functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to measure brain activity while human subjects learn to make Early Stone Age tools. This is the first study to use fNIRS to address paleoanthropological questions, and in many ways it is an ideal neuroimaging technique for observing brain activity during stone tool manufacture.

The results of this study will address the motor theory of language origin, which argues that language developed out of a pre-existing motor system, such as that used by early hominins to make relatively complex stone tools. It is my hope that this research and future experiments will help pave the way to a clearer picture of how and when humans began to communicate and think with language.

Further reading:

"The role of verbal interaction during experimental bifacial stone tool manufacture" - Lithic Technology

"Validating a new methodology for optical probe design and image registration in fNIRS studies"NeuroImage

Posted
AuthorH Gregory

We are pleased to introduce you to another one of our newest grantees, Amanda Lea, PhD candidate from Duke University.  She was awarded a Leakey Foundation research grant in the Fall of 2014 for her project entitled "Effects of social conditions on DNA methylation and immune function."

Amanda Lea

Many primates, including humans, live in complex social environments in which both competitive and affiliative interactions are common. The capacity to deal with this environment can vary substantially across individuals. While some individuals form stable, positive social bonds, others do not; similarly, only some individuals are able to achieve high social status. Research in both humans and nonhuman primates suggests that variation in these social experiences can have profound effects on physiology, health, and survival. However, we do not understand the molecular mechanisms that mediate these effects.

Baboon (Papio cynocephalus) in Amboseli, Kenya

To address this gap, my project tests the hypothesis that social adversity influences health-related traits by altering the way genes are expressed. Specifically, it asks whether low social status and/or social isolation lead to changes in DNA methylation (an environmentally sensitive modifier of gene expression) and whether these changes predict how individuals respond to an immune challenge. To do so, my project combines behavioral data from wild baboons in Amboseli, Kenya, with measurements of genome-wide DNA methylation levels and immune-related gene expression patterns.

Together, the resulting data will shed new light on whether, and to what degree, social experiences influence immunological traits that likely contribute to health and survival in wild primates. Identifying these connections is crucial for understanding the evolution of primate sociality, and for addressing the well-documented health consequences of social adversity in human populations. Finally, by using a genomic approach, I will be able to identify specific genes involved in the response to the social environment, which are likely to have been important in the evolution of group living in primates. 

More information:

Amboseli Baboon Research Project
The Alberts Lab - Duke University
The Tung Lab - Duke University
"The Role of Methylation in Gene Expression" - Nature Education
"Baboon Watch" - Science Magazine

Posted
AuthorH Gregory

Michael Granatosky, PhD candidate from Duke University, was awarded a Leakey Foundation research grant in our most recent granting cycle for his project entitled "Gait mechanics of inverted walking: Implications for evolution of suspensory behavior."

Michael Granatosky at the Duke Lemur Center

Specialized arm-swinging locomotion has arisen independently numerous times during the evolution of primates and yet has never appeared in any other mammalian lineage.  Currently, few theories explaining the transition to specialized arm-swinging locomotion in primates have been proposed, and none of these provide an experimental framework by which to approach this question.  My dissertation focuses on the mechanics of below-branch quadrupedalism, which is a form of suspensory locomotion commonly seen in primates and other mammals that requires few anatomical modifications.  The goal of my project is to understand the proximate strategies mammals use to adjust mechanically to below-branch quadrupedal locomotion and then determine whether below-branch quadrupedal locomotion relates to the ultimate evolution of specialized arm-swinging locomotion in primates. 

To answer these questions, I will collect locomotor data from primates, sloths, and bats during a number of experimental comparisons.  First, I will collect locomotor data from primates walking above and below a runway.  This comparison will determine what aspects of locomotor behavior changes between above and below-branch quadrupedal locomotion.  Next, I will compare the mechanics of below-branch quadrupedal locomotion between primate and non-primate mammals.  This will determine whether there are multiple strategies for adopting below-branch quadrupedal locomotion, or whether all animals are moving in a similar fashion.  Finally, I will compare the mechanics of below-branch quadrupedal locomotion with specialized arm-swinging locomotion in primates to determine if below-branch quadrupedal locomotion may have served as a locomotor precursor to specialized arm-swinging locomotion.   

Posted
AuthorH Gregory

Anne E. Russon
York Univesity

Anne E. Russon

In spring 2012 The Leakey Foundation awarded Anne E. Russon a grant for her long-term study of behavior in east Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus morio) at the Bendili study area (Kutai National Park). This project focused specifically on orangutan ranging, feeding ecology and spatial cognition. 

Researchers have studied orangutans in the Bendili and nearby Mentoko study areas several times over the last 44 years.  During this time their habitat has undergone drastic changes, including commercial logging incursions, severe droughts and massive forest fires. In the report below Russon describes some of her team’s findings, including how intra-site comparisons of activity budgets and travel distances provide insight into how these orangutans coped with this series of habitat changes. 


Posted
AuthorH Gregory
CategoriesResearch Report

It is with profound sadness that we share with you the passing of Brad Goodhart, the devoted husband of the Foundation's Grants Officer Paddy Moore-Goodhart. 

Brad Goodhart and Paddy Moore-Goodhart on one of their many adventures.

Brad Goodhart and Paddy Moore-Goodhart on one of their many adventures.

Brad had an enduring love for Africa's people and nature, having led over 100 tours of East Africa over the past 35 years. He was a Board Member of the African Orphans Foundation where he helped coordinate fundraising for orphaned girls in Kenya, Tanzania, Uganda and Ethiopia. He taught science, math and English at Bishop O’Dowd High School from 1968-1997, and was most recently teaching at Geyserville High School.

His generous smile, insatiable curiosity, and helpful hand made him a special addition to the Foundation family.

He succeeded in living a full life while inspiring so many others to do the same. Please join us in sending our most sincere condolences to Paddy and her family.

A celebration of Brad's life will be held at 2:00pm on Monday, January 19 at Villa Chanticleer in Healdsburg, California. 

Posted
AuthorMeredith Johnson
4 CommentsPost a comment

Over the next few months we will be introducing you to Leakey Foundation grantees from our Fall 2014 granting cycle. Our first featured grantee is Alia Gurtov, PhD candidate from the University of Wisconsin, Madison. Her project is entitled "Dental microwear analysis of Early Pleistocene hominin foraging seasonality." 

Alia Gurtov in Rising Star Cave

For our Early Stone Age predecessors in eastern Africa the changing of the seasons meant a profound shift in the quantity and distribution of food and water. Around 2 million years ago at Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania, the global trend toward increasing aridity had begun, and rainfall was limited to a few short months of the year, much as it is today. The savanna grasslands spread to the detriment of tropical forests and the biome they supported. At the same time, the evidence for meat eating in the Olduvai archaeological record became stronger, with more prey animals preserving butchery evidence than ever before. Under these new ecological conditions, the foraging strategies that our ancestors employed provide critical insights into the process of becoming human. Did Early Stone Age meat foraging vary seasonally? If so, did those activities overlap in time and space with the saber toothed cats that occupied the top of the food chain?

Alia Gurtov, MNI analysis, National Museum of Dar es Salaam, Tanzania

By looking at the microscopic dental abrasions caused by chewing, I have shown that it is possible to determine in which season an assemblage of modern eastern African impala died. With the support of the Leakey Foundation, I will use dental microwear analysis to study the season or seasons of death represented by prey animals at Olduvai archaeological sites. I will compare these seasonal mortality profiles to those produced by contemporaneous carnivores at Olduvai and modern Hadza hunter gatherers in Tanzania. Together, these data will help to answer our questions about Early Stone Age behavioral flexibility as seen through foraging adaptations to a changing and challenging environment. 

More information:

National Museum of Dar es Salaam
The Olduvai Paleoanthropology and Paleoecology Project

Alia Gurtov at Olduvai

Posted
AuthorH Gregory

On December 6th The Leakey Foundation’s Board of Trustees convened for our Fall 2014 Granting Session. The Board unanimously approved the twenty-five research grants our Scientific Executive Committee presented as recommended for funding.  

Here are a few numbers from our Fall 2014 Granting Cycle:

  • There were 75 research grant applications
  • 40% were categorized as behavioral, 60% were paleoanthropology
  • Over 400 reviews were submitted to our grants department during this cycle
  • Of the 25 approved grants, 11 were submitted by PhD candidates

We would like to congratulate all of our new grantees, and we look forward to sharing news and information about them and their research along the way! 

Here are The Leakey Foundation's Fall 2014 Grantees:

Thure Cerling, University of Utah:  Stable isotopes of fossil primates in Kenya

Naomi Cleghorn, University of Texas, Arlington:  Investigating a rare Early Later Stone Age site at Knysna, South Africa

Constance Dubuc, New York University:  Male-male competition, sexual dimorphism and alternative reproductive strategies in a non-human primate: A morphological approach

Alexander Georgiev, University of Chicago:  Male oxidative stress and female mate choice in rhesus macaques

Michael Granatosky, Duke University:  Gait mechanics of inverted walking: Implications for evolution of suspensory behavior

Alia Gurtov, University of Wisconsin, Madison:  Dental microwear analysis of Early Pleistocene hominin foraging seasonality

Andrew Halley, University of California, Berkeley:  Determining the embryonic origins of human and primate encephalization

Amanda Lea, Duke University:  Effects of social conditions on DNA methylation and immune function

Fredrick Manthi, National Museums of Kenya: Investigation of new Pleistocene sites in the Turkana Basin, Kenya

Emily McLean, Duke University:  Direct and indirect genetic effects on social behaviors in baboons

Armand Salvador Mijares, University of the Philippines:  The 2015 archaeological excavation of Callao Cave, northern Luzon, Philippines

Elizabeth Moffett, University of Missouri:  Birth and its effects on anthropoid pelvic shape and integration

David Pappano, Princeton University:  Dynamic network analysis of gelada herding and movement

Marta Pina, Institut Català de Paleontologia Miquel Crusafont:  Hindlimb mechanical properties in Miocene apes: Origins of human locomotion

Samantha Porter, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities:  Investigating cultural transmission across the MP-UP transition in western Europe

Shelby Putt, University of Iowa:  Investigating the co-evolution of language and toolmaking: An fNIRS study

Rhonda QuinnSeton Hall University:  Refining paleosol isotopic evidence from Omo-Turkana hominin environments

Michael Rogers, Southern Connecticut State University:  The Oldowan-Acheulian transition at Gona, Ethiopia: Archaeological and geological studies

Clara Scarry, University of Texas, Austin:  Evolution and maintenance of male cooperation among tufted capuchin monkeys

Gabriele Schino, Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, C.N.R.:  The emotional basis of primate reciprocity

Nicole Squyres, Johns Hopkins University:  Morphological variation in the distal femur of modern humans and fossil hominins

Adam  Sylvester, University of Glasgow:  Reconstructing walking kinematics from femoral condyle curvature in fossil hominins

Maura Tyrrell, University at Buffalo, State University of New York:  Effect of competition on male coalition patterns in crested macaques

Sarie Van Belle, University of Texas at Austin:  Paternity and kinship in socially monogamous saki and titi monkeys

Brian Wood, Yale University:  Stone tools as digging implements: Archaeological, energetic, and biomechanical implications


Posted
AuthorH Gregory

May 2014 (dry season): Geladas come off the cliffs, where they sleep at night, and regroup at the top. They often take an hour or so to socialize and rest before heading off to forage. Pictured (left to right): graduate student Morgan Gustison; field assistant Esheti; field manager Megan Gomery. Photo by E. Tinsley Johnson.

In the spring of 2013 Elizabeth Tinsley Johnson, PhD candidate from the University of Michigan, was awarded a Leakey Foundation research grant for her project entitled "A test of the vocal grooming hypothesis in the gelada." 

Geladas (close relatives of baboons) are exceptionally unique primates that are only found in the highlands of Ethiopia. What makes them unique? First, they are the only primates in the world that eat primarily grass, which means they spend a lot of time foraging – which sometimes comes at the expense of socializing. Maintaining social bonds is a critical part of gelada life, because they live in large, complex social groups. For females, in particular, the ability to maintain ties with other females in their unit is crucial for maintaining unit cohesion. Geladas are also unique in that they are exceptionally vocal primates, leading some researchers to speculate that the function of their constant chatter is to make up for lost grooming time. If true, the so-called ‘Vocal Grooming Hypothesis’ (e.g., Dunbar 1996) carries implications for how, and why, human language evolved.

May 2014 (dry season): A close-up of a family unit grooming (and playing) in the morning. Females spend roughly 20% of their waking hours grooming one another, a behavior that is critical to forming and maintaining social bonds with other members of their unit. Photo by E. Tinsley Johnson.

For the last year (Oct. 2013-Oct. 2014), graduate student Elizabeth Tinsley Johnson has been studying geladas in the Simien Mountains National Park, Ethiopia. Her research aims to understand how female geladas maintain social bonds and the potential benefits these relationships carry. In particular, Tinsley Johnson is interested in the potential role that vocal contact plays in navigating social relationships. To this end, she has collected climatological, demographic, behavioral, and hormonal data on ~45 adult females across 12 social units. Now back at the University of Michigan, Tinsley Johnson will spend the next year analyzing her data and presenting her results to professional conferences and in journal publications.

July 2014 (rainy season): A large band of geladas spends the last minutes of daylight foraging before heading back down to the safety of the cliffs to sleep. Photo by E. Tinsley Johnson.

September 2014 (rainy season): Researchers from the University of Michigan’s Gelada Research Project wait at the top of a sleep site for the geladas to come up for the day. Photo by E. Tinsley Johnson.

September 2014 (rainy season): Graduate student Elizabeth Tinsley Johnson recording the behaviors and vocalizations of female geladas as the band forages during the day. Photo by J. C. Beehner.

Posted
AuthorH Gregory

Paco Bertolani, PhD Candidate
University of Cambridge

The Tshuapa-Lomami-Lualaba area (TL2) lies at the eastern edge of the bonobos’ species range in the Congo basin. In what eventually led to the creation of the TL2 Project, Terese and John Hart organized extensive surveys of TL2 starting in 2007. They confirmed the presence of bonobos in this area, estimating a population of over 10,000 individuals. This relatively pristine, remote habitat had the potential to be an area of extraordinary importance for the future of bonobos.

In a Leakey Foundation research grant awarded in spring of 2011, Paco Bertolani proposed a pilot study to assess the feasibility of establishing a long-term site for bonobo behavioral research and conservation in TL2. He and his team would conduct surveys in search of bonobo traces. This would include detailed mapping of the study area as well as the collection of fecal samples for genetic analysis and surveying the health of the bonobos. They would also monitor signs of hunting and other human disturbance in collaboration with TL2 Project staff.

In the report below, Bertolani summarizes the results of his team’s findings, including his assessment of the feasibility- and riskiness- of establishing a long-term bonobo research site in TL2 where poaching is a serious problem.



Posted
AuthorH Gregory
CategoriesResearch Report

Young blue monkey

Though the wild blue monkey (Cercopithecus mitis) has a social structure of one-male social groups, this organization can be somewhat dynamic. Resident males do not always have a reproductive monopoly over the females, and non-resident (bachelor) males may have a higher reproductive success than one might expect.

Su-Jen Roberts

 

Su-Jen Roberts, PhD candidate from Columbia University, was awarded a research grant in spring of 2012 for her project to assess what variables drive variance in reproductive success in wild blue monkeys. The report below describes how Roberts used DNA analysis to assign paternity to the study population in the Kakamega Forest of Kenya.  She then combined these results with long-term observational data in order to better understand how variables such as female reproductive synchrony or the number and proximity of competitor males affect the reproductive success of resident and bachelor males.   

Posted
AuthorH Gregory
CategoriesResearch Report

Oliver Paine testing specimens on the Kudu Hill transect.

Recent studies have suggested that C4 plants played a larger role in early hominin diets than previously believed, thus making a systematic effort to determine the costs and benefits of C4 plants for hominin consumption that much more important in helping to model early hominin dietary behavior. 

The following is a short update from Leakey Foundation spring 2014 grantee Oliver Paine, University of Colorado at Boulder, who recently completed the first of two trips to the Cradle Nature Reserve (Cradle of Humankind World Heritage Site, South Africa) where he and his team are performing nutritional and mechanical analyses of C4 and other plants in savanna microhabitats across seasons. We look forward to hearing from him again soon.  

 Left to right: Dr. James Louden (East Carolina University), Alex Cowper (CU Boulder), Oliver Paine, Dr. Amanda Henry (Max Planck), Abigail Koppa (Stony Brook), and Jen Leichliter (CU Boulder)

As always, the dry season in the South African Highveld is remarkable with regard to how little palatable food seemingly exists on the landscape. My research partners and I are always amazed by the fact that herbivorous fauna, both large and small, are able to eke out an existence during this time. As such, our collections were precisely what we had expected: plenty of dormant grasses and forbs. However, there were pockets of green vegetation scattered about  and, in particular, around our wetland transects. Additionally, some tree species, such as Searsia (Rhus) lancea, were fruiting in parts of the reserve.

Impala on one of the burned areas

 
The most significant unexpected hurdle we faced was the fact that a month prior to our arrival, the reserve experienced a massive veld fire that charred roughly half of its vegetation. Luckily, only one of our transects was significantly damaged but the "moonscapes" that surrounded us as we drove through the reserve were eerie at times. Such is the nature of fieldwork!

Posted
AuthorH Gregory
CategoriesFrom the Field

The population of white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) in the Sector Santa Rosa (SSR) of the Area de Conservación Guanacaste in northwestern Costa Rica offers a unique opportunity to model how landscape variability affected selection on early hominins.  This species shows many anatomical and behavioral convergences with great apes and humans, and they thrive in a broad range of environmental conditions, including the SSR, which could be considered a microcosm of early hominin environments.

In fall of 2010 Fernando Campos, PhD candidate at the University of Calgary, was awarded a Leakey Foundation research grant for his comprehensive examination of the effects of environmental change on this population of capuchin monkeys.  For this project Campos combined behavioral data collected over a period of 18 months with long-term census and demographic data, satellite imagery and genetic sampling. He summarizes his principle findings in the report below. 

Posted
AuthorH Gregory
CategoriesResearch Report